IJT
Electronic Books
Friedemann Pfäfflin, Astrid Junge
Sex Reassignment. Thirty Years of International Follow-up Studies After Sex Reassignment Surgery: A Comprehensive Review, 1961-1991(Translated from German into American English by Roberta B. Jacobson and Alf B. Meier)
Content
Introduction

Methods
Follow-up Studies
(1961-1991)
Reviews
Table of Overview
Results and Discussion
References

IJT
Current Volume
Search
Linklist

Subscribers
only
book Historic Papers
Electronic Books
Printed Digest

Newsletter

Type in your E-mail address (press Enter) to get the abstracts of every new issue via E-mail.

Info
Authors´Guidelines
Subscription Info

© Copyright

Published by
Symposion Publishing


Chapter 6: Results and Discussion

Treatment Results

General Results
The follow-up studies and reviews conclude that gender reassigning treatments are effective. Positive, resp., desired results overweigh continuously and pregnantly the negative or non-desired effects. The results with males are, on average somewhat more favorable , than those with females. This statement is valid despite the wide spectrum of attitudes of authors about treatment for sex reassignment and disregarding the qualitatively very different support to which the single authors refer to justify their results. For the total results, it is absolutely unimportant how big the reservations of individual authors to start such a treatment were (e.g., Ball, 1981), for how problematic they considered the surgical intervention for the treatment (e.g., Alanko & Achté, 1971) and/or rather would have trusted a psychotherapeutic treatment (e.g., Meyer & Reter, 1979; Lothstein, 1980; McCauley & Ehrhardt, 1984). If they considered the recourse to surgery as ultima ratio (e.g., Täschner & Wiesbeck, 1988) or as the best treatment method (e.g., Steiner, 1976) and method of choice (e.g., Laub & Fisk, 1974). And finally to be considered is how much they doubted and questioned that something like a sex change was at all possible(e.g., Randell, 1969; Sörensen, 1981 a, b; comp. to Hertoft & Sörensen, 1979).

Regarding the global percentages of satisfactory results they range from 71,4% for females and 89,5% for males in the early reviews (Pauly 1968,1971; Lundström et al., 1984) and 87% for females and 97% for males in the newest review that contains only follow-up studies of the last decade (Green & Fleming, 1990). Pauly (1974,a,b) calculated within 35 single case studies from the literature a bettering rate of 100%, arguing convincingly that this high value is due to the selective representation of positive courses and does not represent reality. Without supplying data of his own Lothstein (1982) questioned the high percentage of positive results between 68%-86% of previuos studies. Abramowitz (1986) established a bettering with about two thirds of patients operated.

Regarding the global percentages of unsatisfactory results, resp., of worsening, they range from about 8.1% for females in the early reviews (Pauly, 1981), over 10.3% (Lundström et al., 1984) to 13% (Green & Fleming, 1989). For males, from 6% (Pauly, 1981), over 9.7% (Lundström et al, 1984) to 3% (Green & Fleming, 1990). Abramowitz (1986) found in 7% of the operated and not differentiated by gender patients, severe complications to which he counted, a.o., desire to revert back, psychotic episodes, hospitalization and suicide.

As discussed in relation to certain reviews, these numbers are based on very rough calculations. The individual follow-up studies offer more differentiated views. Improvement as well as worsening are represented. Reported about are shortcomings of the treatment; of incomplete treatment; about passing, in principle, avoidable, as well as permanent, complications. Lindemalm et al. (1986) called the value of 30% as the highest global non-success rate. Because the samples are mostly small and/or heterogeneous the type and extent of the treatment diverged widely and the scope and valuing standards differed greatly, it seems to us to be a problem to add the shared data and to calculate global improvement rates as the authors of some earlier reviews had done.

As part of the global result evaluation in single publications and reviews, many areas are cited in which, in accordance to the scope and research methods, "improvement," "worsening" or "no change" are specified in many areas. In view of the heterogeneity of the original works referred to, we do not deem as useful to compile the data to certain themes. Because of this, we will only highlight those results that can be evaluated as methodological sound. In this, we are interested mostly in three questions: (1) What are the effective factors to which the positive results are related? (2) What are the outstanding areas in which the desired and undesired effects were measured? (3) Can defined groups be differentiated -- diagnostically or by other means -- in which the results attained were more resp. less favorable?

  Effectiveness Factors
The follow-up studies of the 1960s and 70s are not differentiated -- with the exception of one publication by Hoenig et al. (1970 b), which we will treat separately -- in regard to the effectiveness of single elements of treatment. The essential part of the treatment for sex change, whose effectiveness was started to be researched, consisted for them as "the surgery." With regard to "the surgery", all other aspects of treatment were of less importance, even though Alanko & Achté (1971) highlighted with emphasis that surgical treatment alone is therapeutically inefficient. Only at the end of the 1970s was it started to isolate single effectiveness factors from the overall effectiveness and to research them.

Disregarding all methodological shortcomings in their work, the first to demonstrate an independent effectiveness factor were Meyer & Reter (1979), who identified continuous contact with an organized research.program. The guidance in the program at the Johns Hopkins Clinic in Baltimore, about which they reported, was evaluation rather than treatment. Patients who succeeded in having regular contact to the research institute were clearly better as patients who could not maintain such contact. Improvements existed already before and, as such, were independent of surgical treatment.

Similar results were found by Blanchard et al. (1983; 1985), Blanchard (1985) and Blanchard & Steiner (1983) with their first multivariate cross-sectional analysis of Canadian patients in different treatment stages. They demonstrated with large samples that psychological ailments and neuroticisms were significantly reduced in the entire course of the process of gender reassignment. They called the process of gender reassignment "gender reorientation." They developed for each of their researches a slightly different gender reorientation index and demonstrated, to the contrary of the supposition of Meyer & Reter (1979), that the measured positive changes were not merely the effect of time passing (the sole aging or becoming an adult for the examined patients, of whom only a partial sample were treated with hormones or were operated), but the effect of gender reorientation. The appearance and the establishment of oneself in the other gender role, the official name change and the entering into partnerships were important factors that contributed to, besides hormone treatments or surgery, a better psychological adaptation.

These findings were confirmed by the research of Kuiper (1985) and Kuiper and Cohen-Kettenis (1988) of The Netherlands. According to the research, patients who in the frame of a Real -Life-Test and being connected to a treatment facility, have started at least a hormonal treatment or already had undergone at least partial surgery suffered less from gender dysphoria than untreated patients. If the surgical treatment was finished or not, or if it hadn't even started, was not important in regard to the subjective well-being.

According to this, Fahrner et al. (1987) found, in comparing three groups, resp., the evaluation of data obtained in three measuring times, that already during the time of the Real-Life-Test, statistically significant positive changes compared to the time of the start of the treatment manifest themselves. To the contrary to the findings from The Netherlands, in those patients who had finished the medical phase of treatment, e.g., operated patients, additional positive changes were measured in areas that were not considered in the Dutch research.

As other effectiveness factors, counseling and psychotherapy were demonstrated in the research of Lothstein (1980) and McCauley and Ehrhardt (1984) of the U.S.A., that probably were a factor in the research of Meyer & Reter (1979) - only that these authors did not differentiate of what the "contact to a research program" consisted.

The surgical interventions, not surprisingly, were also isolated as an effective factor in the above-mentioned Munich research (Fahrner et al., 1987). The work of Mate-Kole et al. (1990) in England three years later confirmed this finding in comparison group research, which, according to the judgment of all authors of previous studies, was not adequate by therapeutic reasons: Patients who were relatively immediately after the diagnosis operated were at the time of the follow-up study socially more active and showed less neuroticisms than other patients who for a time period of at least two years were kept on a waiting list and were not yet operated.

For a long time the results regarding the question in how far the cosmetic and functional quality of the surgery contributed to the total results was disputed. Generalized thought, single case studies, case studies of the follow-up studies referred here to and clinical impressions spoke for the hypothesis that the total results must be worse the worse the quality of the surgical results were (e.g., Benjamin, 1966, 1967; Hastings, 1974; Eicher, 1984; Lindemalm et al., 1986; compare to these the reference to suicide and regrets as follows). In the same sense some authors report about generally good results that they related to a well-done surgical treatment (e.g., Pomeroy, 1967; Hore et al., 1975; Zingg et al., 1980; Ball, 1981; Eicher, 1984; Eicher et al., 1991). Finally, there are repeatedly indications in the follow-up studies that patients were either, despite "objective" satisfactory surgical results, unhappy (e.g., Lindemalm et al., 1986); resp., the other way around, in spite of "objective" unsatisfactory surgical results, were happy with the results of surgery (e.g., McEwan et al., 1986; Blanchard et al., 1987). Already Randell (1969) had observed that satisfaction, resp., dissatisfaction, with the gender change was independent of the ability to function sexually. In our own follow-up research we could not secure a relationship between the quality of the surgical results and the satisfaction (Pfäfflin & Junge, 1990). The contradiction of these impressions and results is probably due to the diagnostic and/or psychopathological and non-homogeneous samples, or it depends on the length and quality of the previous psychiatric/psychotherapeutic treatment and the measure in which the possibility has been worked out that the surgical results can stay far beyond the desired. It was Ross & Need (1989) who demonstrated with research of a psychopathological homogeneous group with the same pre-treatment that an important part of the variation in a general judgment depends on the quality of the surgical results.

Long before the mentioned effectiveness factors were isolated, Hoenig et al. (1970b) had tested the influence of a juridical treatment measure (comp. Will, 1992), namely the name change and the change of the National Insurance Card and found that patients with a name change found work easier, stayed in the same job longer and had less legal sentencing encounters with the law. Even if the differences were not significant statistically, they are equal to the impression of other follow-up studies in which the positive or negative consequences of name and legal sex changes were referred to, depending if such were possible in the respective national laws (e.g., Gunn-Sechehaye, 1964; Vogt, 1968; Walser, 1968; Wålinder & Thuwe, 1975; Stürup, 1976; Tsoi et al., 1978; Wyler, 1978; Ball, 1981; Pfäfflin & Junge, 1990). In the studies of Blanchard (1985) regarding the process of gender reorientation, the name change represents an important effectiveness factor.

According to this, seven factors could be isolated that contribute to the effect and effectiveness to treatment of gender reassignment. Specifically (1) continuous contact with a research program or a treatment center; (2) living in the other gender role (Real-Life-Test); (3) hormone treatment; (4) counseling, psychiatric and/or psychotherapeutic treatment; (5) surgical sex reassigning treatments, as well as (6) their quality; and finally; 7) the juridical recognition of the gender change by names and sex.

There is no statistically secure knowledge about how long the first four factors had to be minimally to give a good result. Control studies about variations of time periods regarding this do not exist until now. As already mentioned in the section about hormonal and surgical treatments, most follow-up studies did not sufficiently differentiate how long the patients had arranged their lives to be recognized as a member of the other gender before beginning their hormone treatment and how long they were treated psychiatricially/psychothreapeutically and hormonally. With regard to the Real-Life-Test, one can only learn from the works that the length required of it diverge significantly. Following the selection criteria of the research and/or its indication as shortest time period between one and three years is mostly required, in one case even five years (Stone, 1977). But it is exactly Stone's work that is exemplary for many other studies that sum-up under Real-Life-Test everything that does not regard immediately genital surgery e.g. the hormone treatment and cosmetic surgery. Finally, the research of Mate-Kole et al. (1990) does not give an answer to the question for successful time periods for the treatment elements despite of three measuring time points. This study only demonstrates that another postponement of the surgical treatment with the indication done by the rules of the art demonstrated to be less favorable than a comparison with an immediately following surgery.

Desired Changes
In the follow-up studies as represented in detail in section 3, treatment results and changes are measured and evaluated under very different points of view and with a wide spectrum of criteria and methods. In the following, the results and evaluation are not considered individually but are compiled into important areas in which, with the follow-up studies, an effect had been demonstrated. These effects can be ordered under a gross simplification in four overlapping areas, namely subjective satisfaction/unsatisfaction; mental stability/instability; socio-economic functioning level and finally, partnership and sexual experience.

Probably the most important change that is found in most research is the increase of subjective satisfaction. It contrasts markedly to the subjectively unsatisfactory start position of the patients. To describe the subjective satisfaction authors have chosen different concepts. Some authors talked about happiness (e.g., Benjamin, 1966; Kuiper & Cohen-Kettenis, 1988); others talked about improved patients or that patients were feeling better. This was a global finding that, in the first two decades in a general way, just was admitted and only regarding and using the improvement of the scales of Hastings (1974) as well as Hunt & Hampson (1980 a) could be measured gradually and specified for the different living situations. Regarding the increment of the subjective satisfaction, all referred-to research agree, even those authors who spoke most critically about the effects of gender reassignment (Meyer & Reter, 1979; Lindemalm et al., 1986).

In accordance to the opinion of the authors of the newest review (Green & Fleming, 1990), the decisive effect of the treatment of gender reassignment is the subjective satisfaction and that is why the authors consider it sufficient criteria. Other authors who also consider this criterion of central importance consider it necessary to compare it with other objective criteria (e.g., Kuiper & Cohen-Kettenis, 1988). The objectivization of satisfaction was done by regarding categories such as mental stability, psychiatric symptoms, clinical or test psychologically measurable psychopathological characteristics, neuroticisms, number of professional psychiatric contacts and hospitalizations, suicidal tendencies, alcohol and drug consumption, etc. One could add all this to the frequently used concepts of psycho-social adaptation and/or function.

If not in the same measure and not as uniform as the subjective satisfaction for the total complex of mental stability can be determined that, in the majority of follow-up research, that include this area found also more positive than negative results. Marked or statistically significant were changes regarding mental stability (e.g., Money & Ehrhardt, 1970; Hastings, 1974; Laub & Fisk, 1974; Wålinder & Thuwe, 1975; Sadoughi et al., 1978; Spengler, 1980; Kröhn et al., 1981; Sörensen, 1981 a; Fahrner et al., 1987; Mate-Kole et al., 1990; Pfäfflin & Junge, 1990). Therein the exclusively test psychological analysis has not been regarded (comp. the overviews in Lothstein, 1984; Pfäfflin 1993.

There is also research that could expressively find no statistically relevant changes regarding mental stability and related phenomena (e.g., Money & Brennen, 1968; Hunt & Hampson, 1980 b) or could find this only in certain partial populations (e.g., Walser, 1968; Stürup, 1976) or only for certain partial aspects, while other partial aspects, for example, alcoholism, stayed the same (e.g., Wålinder & Thuwe, 1975). While one can read in between the lines of some authors that patients were as stable after treatment, the more stable the personality at the outset had been (e.g., Walser, 1968). Lundström denies this (1981), explaining that the psycho-sexual functioning level of the starting personality is not of major importance for the success of the treatment; or if there is, it is so extremely low that improvement was not generally expected. The themes suicidal tendencies and role reversal that belong to the bigger complex mental stability will be discussed because of their special importance further on.

Positive results are affirmed by most follow-up studies regarding the socio-economic functioning level. Under this category the ability to contact in partnerships, with relatives, in the neighborhood and at the work place are compiled, as well as the socio-economic improvements. Improvements of the social functional level based on clinical judgement or by means of statistically significant comparison data are reported in the works of Wålinder (1967); Vogt (1968); Walser (1968); Randell (1968); Money & Ehrhardt (1970); Hoenig et al. (1970 b); Ihlenfeld (1973); Hastings (1974); Steiner (1976); Stone (1977); König et al., (1978); Turner et al. (1978); Meyer & Reter (1979); Hunt & Hampson (1980 b); Lothstein (1980); Ball (1981); Fahrner et al. (1987); Kockott & Fahrner (1988); Mate-Kole et al. (1990); Pfäfflin & Junge (1990) and Eicher et al. (1991). The measure of the registered changes differs considerably from study to study and even within one research, depending on the regarded partial aspects of the very ample criteria here. Hoenig et al. (1979b) found, for example, improvement in the work situation and the diminishing of the crime rate, but the treatment had no influence on the measure of being dependent on national assistance. Discrepancies of the mostly good results of the mentioned publications were found in the sample of females described by Sörensen (1981 a), mostly social decline and more social isolation but not in the described sample of males described by the same author (Sörensen, 1981 b). This difference is in accordance with the majority findings that the partial samples of males find their way better generally than females.

The fourth major area regards partnerships and sexual experience. In some samples these are discussed in the follow-up studies in regard to satisfaction, unsatisfaction and/or the psycho-social function level - and without doubt there are overlaps. Many follow-up studies indicate if there were -- before and/or after treatment marriages -- and how long they lasted. The simple fact of such a marriage or divorce does not say anything about the quality of the experience. The marriages contracted after finishing the treatment are to be regarded only in the formal, not in the qualitative sense, as a result of the treatment. The stability and quality of the partnerships entered into in the course or following treatment are mentioned especially by Wålinder & Thuwe (1975); Blanchard & Steiner (1983) and Kockott & Fahrner (1988). The two last mentioned follow-up studies agreed that males were more capable than females to maintain stable and satisfactory partnerships. Apart from specific follow-up study literature, partnerships of persons with transsexual symptoms were researched in the publications of Studer et al. (1989); Huxley et al. (1981); Steiner & Bernstein (1981) and Steiner (1985).

More important seems to be the sexual experience that, undoubtedly, represents an aspect of the general subjective satisfaction/dissatisfaction, but was researched separately in some follow-up studies. In his first follow-up study Benjamin (1964 a) did not include sexual satisfaction or orgasmic experience in the evaluation areas because he regarded answers regarding this area by women as unreliable. Also Pomeroy (1967), who made the question of sexual experience the central part of his follow-up study, distrusted the answers of females to the orgasmic experience because he could not fit them into his physiological image. In later follow-up studies such information was taken more seriously (e.g., Wålinder (1967); Hastings (19749; Laub & Fisk (1974); Steiner (1976); Hunt & Hampson (1980); Lothstein (1980); Eicher (1984); Blanchard et al. (1987); Kröhn et al. (1981); Fahrner et al. (1987); Ross & Need (1989); Pfäfflin & Junge (1990); Eicher et al. (1991). The four last mentioned follow-up studies found that the sexual experience improved statistically significantly and that most females experienced orgasms regularly. Worthy of attention is that with males, despite far more limited surgical possibilities in the genital area, the sexual satisfaction after treatment was statistically significantly higher than before treatment (e.g., Junge, 1987).

Undesired Effects, Complications, Risks
Complicated and undesired effects can happen in every stage and regarding every type of treatment. Because hormonal and psychiatric-psychothreapeutic treatment is generally documented only in fragments one cannot find a valid image regarding this in the follow-up study reports. An exception is the most severe psychiatric complication, the suicide, that is discussed following the complications of the surgical treatment. Overviews about complications about hormonal treatment are, apart from the specific follow-up study literature, in Asscheman (1989); Asscheman et al. (1988 a, b); Lips et al. (1989). About single complicated courses in connection with the difficulty or impossibility to change name or personal data report, for example, Gunn-Sechehaye (1964); Stürup (1976); Wyler (1978) and Ball (1981).

Complications Within Surgical Treatment. It is difficult to draw a line between that counting as complication within an unfinished treatment and what has to be evaluated as a more-or-less bad treatment result. It is readily agreeable that a treatment of an MFT is not finished if only the penectomy and orchidectomy was done, but no vaginal surgery had been initiated and with FMT if no matectomy was done. It is more difficult if one regards the formerly frequently used intervals between the surgeries done at different times. In such cases the unfinished treatment and the impossibility to change name and legal sex can be a complication in itself or other further complications that express themselves in partnerships, in the workplace or also in the psychopathological symptoms (for example, suicidal tendencies). This is also valid for the penile surgery that is desired by many FMT even if they follow the advice of their treatment provider to wait until the success expectation for such surgery is better.

The spectrum of the immediate complications from surgery is widely dispersed. Some authors mention intra- or immediately post-surgical complications such as haematomas, abscess forming, bleeding, infections, etc., that have to be viewed as general surgical complications and not as specific complications in sex reassignment surgery.

Others indicate specific complications in MFT operations, among which necrosis and partial necroses of the vagina and labia are found, as well as fistulae in the bladder and intestines, stenosis in the end of the urinal tract and finally too-short and/or to-small vaginas that principally require additional surgery. From surgical breast enlargements, infections and capsular fibrosis are mentioned.

Among the worst complications in FMT operations for breast reduction are nipple necrosis and ugly scarring. With clitoris-penoid and phallus operations there are urinary tract stenosis and fistulae and, also necrosis of theneo-phallus. Besides this, there can be rejection reactions the silastic-testicle prosthesis and to a dehiscence of the scrotal suturemay occur.

If these complications cannot be resolved by follow-up surgery, either because it cannot be done technically or because the patient does not want any more surgery, then the surgery must be valued as unsatisfactory, resp., bad results. Already Benjamin noted in some of his publications that in his samples there were series of complications that could have been solved principally so that the results in general could have been much better if the necessary follow-up surgery had been performed. McEwen et al. (1986) said that the rate of complications was 35%, but also said that most complications could be solved. It is important to inform the patients that the results are not always perfect.

By far, not in all follow-up study reports in which complications are mentioned at all are there specific indications. In some works, it is only mentioned globally that there were many complications and/or many corrective treatments (e.g., Benjamin, 1964 b; Money & Ehrhardt, 1970; Wyler, 1978; Sörensen, 1981 a; Pfäfflin & Junge, 1990). Stone (1977) even said that most patients of his sample had complications. Other works quantify at least some of the severe complications (e.g., Benjamin, 1966; Zingg et al., 1980; Kröhn et al., 1981; Wiegand, 1984; Dudle, 1989). Eicher (1984) reported unsatisfaction regarding the local finding and interference of the sexual capabilities of those 10% of females of his sample who suffered from vaginal stricture and/or shortening and added that these females, where the local finding was revised successfully, were satisfied afterwards.

One older work, Laub & Fisk (1974), reported over 40% complications in those females operated by them, but also counted, for example, the necessity of blood transfusions as complications. By comparison, they found in females primarily operated elsewhere a complications quota of 92%. This frighteningly high value is due to the effect of the sample that almost exclusively patients who had complications in their first surgery came (to them) for secondary surgeries. A similar comparison was done by Jayaram et al. (1978). Hore et al. (1975) reported exclusively about secondary surgeries and the removal of previously caused complications and improvements of bad surgical results.

Suicide Rate: In 39 of the follow-up studies and reviews referred to here, the suicide rate of patients is themed. The spectrum reaches from very global indications about quantifications that do not distinguish enough between depressive moods, thoughts of suicide, threatening suicides, suicidal attempts and suicides, to very specific indication regarding the respective sample. Alanko & Achté (1971) estimated that the risk of suicide in transsexualism is very high. According to Hastings (1974, p. 335) "most" of the patients compiled in his follow-up study had attempted suicide. In the group analyzed by Wålinder (1967) he found in the anamnesis in 68% of the patients depressive moods, in 49% suicidal thoughts and in 16% suicide attempts. All these indications refer to patients before or at the beginning of treatment.

Regarding the suicidal tendencies after surgery, the specifications of these indications in the follow-up studies and reviews are just as widely dispersed: According to Hunt & Hampson (1980 b), many said spontaneously and with great emphasis that they were only alive because of the sex reassignment. Very vaguely quantified, Hastings (1974) reported something similar, namely, that every second female, in accordance to her subjective judgement, was saved from suicide by the surgery. Kröhn et al. (1981) spoke of a blatant diminishing of pre-suicidal tendencies, but only quantified the frequency of post-surgical suicidal thoughts reported.

Three works contain exclusively general indications about suicidal tendencies. In seven works only indications to pre-surgical suicidal tendencies can be found, in 11 publications exclusively corresponding indications for the katamnesis time period. Pre-post comparisons are contained in 13 works. Because of sample overlapping the total (number) of follow-up study subjects cannot be exactly ascertained. Because of this, only something approximate about the magnitude of suicide in patients with transsexual symptoms can be deduced. Corresponding intents were made by Pauly (1965, 1981) and Lundström et al. (1984) in their reviews, with the result that the post-operative suicide rate for females is 1.9% and for males, 0.8%. (Lundström et al., 1984; in where also they counted the psychotic patients described by Stürup in 1976 who were not operated and cases that, in accordance to the judgement of the primary authors -- Randell (1969) and Wålinder & Thuwe (1974) -- were not or only circumstantially in a causal connection with the sex reassignment.)

To determine the pre-surgical suicidal tendency the use of a bigger sample description independent of follow-up literature is useful. Among 479 MFT and 285 FMT who were examined in the Gender Disphoria Program in Palo Alto, California (USA), 25.5% of the MFT and 19% of the FMT immediately before or a longer time before the treatment started had attempted suicide (Dixen et al., 1984). In the same dimensions are the figures from The Netherlands. According to them, of 168 MFT, 19.3% had attempted suicide before treatment start and of 55 MFT, 18.6% had tried it prior to treatment (Verschoor & Poortinga, 1988). In 18 of the follow-up studies referred to here are indications about the pre-operative suicidal tendencies. In a major number of publications, it is around 20% or even higher (e.g., Hoenig et al., 1971; Wålinder & Thuwe, 1975; Wyler, 1978; Sörensen, 1981 a, b; Kuiper & Cohen-Kettenis, 1988; Dudle, 1989; Wiesbeck & Täschner, 1989; Stein et al., 1990). Throughout all, one can find the difference between the relatively high suicidal tendency of the MFT and the relatively low one of the FMT. An exception is the group of FMT described by McCauley & Ehrhardt (1984), in which 6 of 15 (40%) of patients had attempted suicide before treatment start. This particularity is probably due to the fact that the authors, in long-time therapies, received more information regarding this than other researchers who could refer only to files or one-time follow-up examinations. This could be an indication that suicide attempts, in reality, happen more frequently than indicated in follow-up studies.

In publications that describe courses that are from further back, suicide threat and self-mutilation to achieve the surgery play an important role (e.g., Hastings, 1974; Stürup, 1976; Wyler, 1978). The only author who declares such threats and also extortion attempts as explicit reasons for removal from the treatment program is Ball (1981). Two authors reported about patients who attempted suicide one or more times as a reaction to being told that sex reassignment surgery was not an adequate measure for them (König et al., 1971; Lundström, 1981).

Suicide was committed by four MFT after the start of treatment but prior to surgery (Stürup, 1976; Spengler, 1980; Eicher, 1974; Wiegand, 1984) and five FMT (Sörensen, 1981 b; Lundström, et al, 1981). Two authors contrast these indications explicitly by emphasizing that, among the operated of their samples, no more suicides were committed (Sörensen, 1981 b; Eicher, 1984). One suicide of FMT (Lundström, 1981), resp., one of MFT (Spengler, 1980, personal report) were seen as immediate reactions to the announcement that admittance to surgery would not be forthcoming. One patient had abandoned the desire for the surgery and, because of a paranoid psychosis, committed suicide (Stürup, 1976). In three other deaths it cannot be discerned from the source if they had already been operated or not (Kuiper & Cohen-Kettenis, 1988).

Eighteen works report about suicide attempts after surgery. Insofar as motives for it are mentioned, primarily, crises in the relationships/partnerships is mentioned (e.g., Kando, 1973; Hastings, 1974; Stürup, 1976; Hunt & Hampson, 1980b). In second place are, especially with males, bad results of the penis operation, resp., the circumstance that such were not done (Money & Ehrhardt, 1970; Hoenig et al., 1971). Sörensen (1981b) describes, as part of his follow-up sample, a man who was not approved for the surgery because he was pre-psychotic. Then he underwent surgery somewhere else and six months later, tried to commit suicide with pills. Some authors say how many of the patients with post-operative suicidal tendencies attempted suicide before (Wålinder & Thuwe, 1975; Kuiper & Cohen-Kettenis, 1988). Other authors remark that patients who had suicidal tendencies before surgery reacted in later partnership crises only with depression but not with suicide (Wyler, 1978; Spengler, 1980) or that suicidal tendencies were observed only in the year after surgery or as long as bad surgical results were not corrected, but after that disappeared (Lothstein, 1980; Stein et al., 1990) .

There are reports of a total of 16 deaths that happened after surgery, in which it is questionable if each was a suicide and if the reason was connected to transsexualism (Benjamin, 1964b; Randell, 1969; Gandy, 1973; Ihlenfeld, 1973; Wålinder & Thuwe, 1975; Sörensen, 1975a; Blanchard et al., 1985; Lindemalm et al, 1986; McEwan et al., 1986); in these works there are actually reports about 17 deaths, but one of the females mentioned by Benjamin is also mentioned by Ihlenfeld (comp. the section about sample overlaps). There are 14 females and one male (Ihlenfeld, 1973) and one patient with not gender specified (Gandy, 1973).

In accordance with the estimate to the primary authors, it is questionable in five cases if they were suicides or accidental medication or drug overdoses (Benjamin, 1964b; Ihlenfeld, 1973). Of the remaining 11 cases, three were in no immediate relation with the problems of gender identity, in the judgment of the primary authors, but were in relation to either the very large surgical complications of a patient who already had suicidal tendencies (Wålinder & Thuwe, 1975), with major complications after operations that did not have anything to do with gender reassignment (Blanchard et al., 1985), or with the loss of work and a partnership crisis (McEwan et al, 1986). With two other females, additional psychiatric factors had a decisive role (Randell, 1969). These two females, as well as another female (Sörensen, 1981a) and a single male (Ihlenfeld, 1973) had undergone surgery against the specific votum of the treating physicians who did not deem the surgery as indicated, but had undergone surgery abroad or in other treatment facilities. The remaining four patients about whose motives nothing is know: One suicide was committed shortly after the surgery (Lindemalm et al, 1986); one in contrast to the impression of the last meeting of the examining physician with the patient (Gandy, 1973); and with two females it is rermarked that they did not belong to the core group of transsexuals, were relatively old at beginning of treatment and were judged prognostically as rather critical (Sörensen, 1981a).

The data can be compiled in the direction that suicide attempts are frequent in patients with transsexual symptoms and that about every fifth patient has tried suicide before starting treatment at least once. As a tendency the rate is higher with MFT than with FMT. This difference is found for the anamnesis, as well as for the treatment and katamnesis. In the course of treatment, the relative number of suicide attempts decreases in accordance with the agreed-upon judgments of the authors who make differentiated statements. Which treatment steps (psychiatric-psychotherapeutic, hormonal, surgical treatment) have a positive effect cannot be differentiated based on the figures alone.

The numbers of reported suicides in the follow-up studies are, in sum, about twice as high for post-operative patients compared to pre-operative patients - but not the suicide attempts. This result is due, primarily, to a distortion of the samples. Mostly in the publications the course of a partial sample of the operated was examined because patients who were not treated in accordance to their wishes in treatment facilities went to other treatment facilities and were lost for the follow-up studies. Reliable data about what happened to them long-term and especially how the lethal suicidal tendency was in this group is hardly available (exception, for example, Lundström, 1981). Besides this, it is to be regarded that with increasing age, the possibility increases that a suicide attempt ends lethally (Häfner, 1991). Finally, the examination of the case studies show that post-operative suicides had decisive factors other than gender identity problems or that the sex reassignment surgery was considered as not indicated by experienced examiners or that the treatment was not sufficient.

On the other hand, it would be inadequate to make the indication for gender reassignment only under the aspect of the suicide prophylaxis. A corresponding styled case is reported by Herschkowitz & Dickes (1978) to exemplify the treatment did not do what it promised. Simplification of this type can be abused easily in social-political discussions, for example, the question if the health insurances have to cover treatment costs (e.g, Ehrhardt, 1985, citing Herschkowitz & Dickes, 1978). There is no question that, in the treatment of highly depressed and suicidal patients with transsexual symptoms, patients have to be treated psychotherapeutically and psychiatrically before the indication for sex reassignment surgery is made. (Wålinder, 1967 and Eicher, 1984 found depressive crises in the anamnesis of over 60% of patients examined by them; comp. Langer, 1985.)

Role-reversal / regrets: If one sums all cases of "relapses" in all follow-up studies, 20 MFT can be found (Hertz et al, 1961; Benjamin, 1966; Walser, 1968; Randell, 1969; Alanko & Achté, 1971; Hastings, 1974; Wålinder & Thuwe, 1975; Stürup, 1976; Wålinder et al, 1978; Ball, 1981; Kröhn et al, 1981; Lindemalm et al., 1986) and five FMT (Benjamin, 1966; Money & Ehrhardt, 1970; König et al, 1978; Meyer & Reter, 1979; McCauley & Ehrhardt, 1984). As already said with suicidal tendencies, figures cannot be made in percents, because the numbers of examined patients cannot be determined exactly due to sample overlaps. Insofar as case descriptions exist, they do however allow a differentiated viewpoint.

Role-reversal / rgrets in MFT: First, it is to be determined that, of the 20 patients, two are described twice (Wålinder & Thuwe, 1975; Wålinder et al., 1978). Two other patients had only begun hormonal treatments and were not operated, not belonging to the real "relapses;" one of them had a religious conversion (Hastings, 1974); another one abandoned the wish for surgery and developed a paranoid psychosis and, in its course, committed suicide (Stürup, 1976). As "passing" repentance cases with short, single or intermittent cases of role-reversal, two patients are described (Ball, 1981; Kröhn et al, 1981), where the case described explicitly by Ball (1981) special inheritance rights that greatly complicated the circumstances. In one case it is only mentioned that the patient had the intention to live as a male, but left it open if really done (Benjamin, 1966).

In examination of the other 14 cases, three factors contributed greatly to the "relapses." The first factor was the differential diagnostic indication for surgical treatment; the second is tightly related to the contextual important criterion if the life in the desired gender role was rehearsed long enough in the Real-Life-Test; finally, the third regards the extent and quality of the surgical procedures . We will regard the first mentioned factor first.

At least four patients developed psychoses or paranoid reactions (Hertz et al., 1961; Alanko & Achté, 1971; Hastings, 1974; Wålinder & Thuwe, 1978), in which it has to be asked if this development could not have been foreseen with a sufficiently long pre-operative psychiatric treatment, especially because the patient described by Hastings (1974) did not fulfill in any way the normal indication criteria. He was noticed more than once for violent crimes, was an alcoholic, had tried to cut off his penis with a shaving blade, castrated himself later and tried to extort vaginal surgery with a threat of suicide. Also one of the patients described by Stürup (1976) had multiple auto-castration attempts behind him, that, according to Springer(1981), were more likely a psychotic than a transsexual development. His treatment was contradictory: First, he was given male, and later, female hormones. Surgically only a castration was done, but no vagina was formed. The patient had not mentioned that he was married, so he could not attain a name change and lived as a female with male identification papers. At the time of the follow-up study, 19 years after the operation, he lived again as a male but was convinced that the operation was right and only regretted that it had been performed so late in his life.

In at least three patients, the requirement of the adaptation to everyday life experience in the opposite gender role was not regarded. They were operated without ever testing the other gender role in their lives (Walser, 1968; Alanko & Achté, 1961). The already mentioned psychotic patient described by Hastings (1974) would also belong to this group.

As a third important factor that can conduce to relapses, the authors mentioned the unsatisfactory application of surgical possibility and unsatisfactory surgical results. Two patients were only orchidectomized and the penises remained, a vaginal surgery was not done and the patients were bitter that they could not achieve more (Walser, 1968; Stürup, 1976). With others, up to five years passed between the orchidectomy or penectomy and the vaginal operation (Hertz et al., 1961; Alanko & Achté, 1971). Insofar as the surgical results are described, they are in almost all "relapses" unsatisfactory (Hertz et al, 1961; Benjamin, 1966; Randell, 1969; Wålinder & Thuwe, 1975; Lindemalm et al, 1986).

Role-reversal in FMT: With high probability, one of the five patients is reported about twice (Money & Ehrhardt, 1970; Meyer & Reter, 1979). The patient is one who circumvened the relevant treatment facility to obtain the surgery. Multiple penis surgeries were unsuccessful, resp., had miserable results. The patient was drug-addicted and repeatedly suicidal and lived sometimes as a male, sometimes as a female. According to Meyer & Reter (1979), the patient only wanted the failure-of-a-penis removed, but did not question the other steps of the gender reassignment. The citing of the other patients is very sparse. One regretted the surgery, but it is not clear if he, like the patient described by McCauley & Ehrhardt (1984), returned to the original gender role (König et al., 1978). One patient had tried to live as a male after surgery without success. This patient had later a breast enlargement surgery (Benjamin. 1966).

A publication of Wålinder et al. (1978) dedicated previously referred-to work especially to the theme relapses, resp. regrets. The authors wanted to isolate the unfavorable factors that were in the personality of the patient or in the social environment. They highlighted that only the amassment, but not the existence of single prognostic unfavorable factors, was a contra-indication for surgery and the differentiated viewing of the single case had precedence. Citing symposium presentations at the 5th International Gender Dysphoria Symposium (1977) -- that are not accessible to us -- Sigusch et al. (1979) mentioned the number of a total of 44 "relapses" - exactly twice as many as we could find in the follow-up studies. There were no extensive and differentiating indications as to the stadium of treatment that the patients were in, if and which indication criteria were regarded, if the repentance was in crisis-like stressful situations, or if it was a permanent role-rereversal situation or even an attempt to reverse the surgery.

According to the aforementioned follow-up study literature, "relapses" are very rare, in FMT even more so than in MFT. In light of these case samples, the impression was gained that in most cases -- with sufficiently careful differential diagnostic (testing), sufficiently long tests of life in the other gender role and a better use of surgical possibilities -- role-rereversal could have been avoided. It seems to us worthy of mention that single patients who were operated with disregard of these three factors and later experienced themselves again as members of their original gender, worked through these painful detours in a way they could stabilize themselves (comp. examples in Benjamin, 1966; Alanko & Achté, 1971; Wålinder & Thuwe, 1975; Stürup, 1976).