Sexuality and Transsexuality:

A New Introduction to Autogynephilia

By Anne Lawrence, M.D.



It's been seven years, and y'know what?
I still get a thrill
when I look at myself in the mirror
and see girl, not boy.
- Kate Bornstein, The Seven Year Itch

CONTENTS:

  1. Preface
  2. Introduction
  3. Sexuality and Transsexuality
  4. Blanchard's Theory
  5. Two Types of Transsexuals
  6. Transsexuality as a Paraphilia
  7. Autogynephilia and SRS
  8. NEW: Critiques of Blanchard's Theory
  9. NEW: Critique: Autogynephilia as "Motive"
  10. NEW: Critique: Autogynephilia in Androphiles
  11. NEW: Critique: Gender Non-Conformity in Autogynephiles
  12. NEW: Critique: Gender Dysphoria Preceding Autogynephilia
  13. NEW: Blanchard's Theory in Perspective
  14. More Support for the Theory
  15. Why Has Autogynephilia Been Ignored?
  16. Is Autogynephilia an "Acceptable" Motive?
  17. NEW: Spiritual Implications of Blanchard's Theory
  18. References


Preface

In October, 1998, I web-published an essay called "Men Trapped in Men's Bodies: An Introduction to the Concept of Autogynephilia." My intention was to introduce a theory, formulated by psychologist Ray Blanchard, that I had found to be personally meaningful during my own transition. My essay quickly became a topic of intense controversy among transsexuals and other transgendered persons. Some found my description of Blanchard's ideas to be enlightening, even liberating. They reported relief at having at last found a theory that described their experiences in comprehensible terms. But others felt mischaracterized by Blanchard's theory, or by my explanation of it. They declared that Blanchard's formulation, which categorized gender dysphoric males as either "autogynephilic" or "homosexual," most definitely did not describe their experiences. Many were angered and offended by my public advocacy of a theory that seemed to deny or devalue their feelings and identities. The reactions of this latter group left me feeling defensive, perplexed, and deeply troubled.

After eighteen months of reading, listening, and reflecting on these issues, I feel it is time to revise my essay. I have come to believe that my original exposition suffered from a lack of balance and critical perspective. Although I stand by much of what I originally wrote, there is also much I would now change. This revision will probably still displease some people; but at least it will provide a more accurate summary of my current view of Blanchard's theory.

Introduction

Male-to-female transsexuals are popularly characterized as "women trapped in men's bodies," i.e., male-bodied persons who are very feminine, overtly or otherwise. And clearly there are some transsexuals for whom this is an accurate description. But there are also some of us male-to-female transsexuals to whom this popular description does not so obviously apply. Many of us transitioned in our 30's, 40's, or even later, after having lived outwardly successful lives as men. Often we were not especially feminine as children, and some of us are not especially feminine after transition, either. Many of us were, or occasionally still are, married to women; often we have fathered children. A sizable number of us identify as lesbian or bisexual after reassignment. Many of us have a past or current history of sexual arousal in association with cross- dressing or cross-gender fantasy. Yet there is no doubt that we experience gender dysphoria -- a term that denotes dissatisfaction with the sexed body -- as intensely as our more outwardly feminine transsexual sisters. And we pursue sex reassignment surgery every bit as avidly, too.

It is not difficult to understand why biologic males who have been extremely feminine ever since childhood, and who are sexually attracted to men, would seek sex reassignment surgery. It is more difficult to understand why those of us who are attracted to women, who have been fairly successful as men, and who do not appear remarkably feminine would do so. What force could be powerful enough to make us give up our whole place in the world -- to make us risk estrangement from our families, loss of our jobs, and rejection by our friends?

Some of us eventually discover that there is one force within ourselves sufficiently powerful to sweep aside all objections. For some of us, it is a force whose daemonic intensity has haunted and bedeviled us nearly all our lives. That force is, of course, sexual desire -- our sexual desire to feminize our bodies.

Sexuality and Transsexuality

Naturally, it flies in the face of conventional wisdom to assert that transsexuality has anything to do with sexual desire. It is widely believed that, while sexual preference may be about sex, transsexuality is about gender. Supposedly we transsexuals transition because we are uncomfortable with our assigned gender role, and we desire to live in the opposite gender role. In fact, in some circles, it is no longer politically correct to talk about "sex reassignment surgery"; the preferred term is now "gender reassignment surgery" or even "gender confirmation surgery." This is to emphasize -- according to the conventional wisdom -- that the quest for the sought-after gender role is primary, and that genital surgery is not an end in itself, but is merely a confirmation of the desired gender transition.

Some people, however, are inclined to doubt the conventional wisdom. Some are clinicians working with transsexual clients, who find that the conventional wisdom often fails to explain much of what they observe. And some are transsexuals themselves -- unusually candid transsexuals, who are not shy about saying that sexual desire was a significant motivation in their transitions. To understand what these people are getting, it is helpful to become acquainted with a seven-syllable word derived from Greek: "autogynephilia."

Blanchard's Theory

The term autogynephilia was coined in 1989 by Ray Blanchard, a clinical psychologist at the Clarke Institute of Psychiatry in Toronto. Blanchard defined autogynephilia as "the propensity to be sexually aroused by the thought or image of oneself as a woman." In a remarkable series of papers (Blanchard, 1985, 1988, 1989a, 1989b, 1991, 1993a, 1993b, 1993c; Blanchard et al, 1987), he explored the role of autogynephilia in the erotic lives of hundreds of male gender dysphoric patients.

Like many researchers before and since, Blanchard was interested in studying the typology of male-to-female transsexualism. Clinicians had observed for years that males who sought sex reassignment were not a homogeneous group. Several different categories of male-to-female transsexualism had been proposed, invariably based on sexual orientation, history of sexual arousal to crossdressing, or a combination of these. Many observers had noted that gender dysphoric males nearly always displayed at least one of two statistically uncommon erotic preferences: either sexual attraction to males, or a history of sexual arousal to crossdressing or cross-gender fantasy.

Based on his research, Blanchard concluded that there were really only two distinct categories of gender dysphoric males: an androphilic group, those who were sexually aroused primarily by males; and a non- androphilic group, those who were sexually aroused primarily by the idea of being women, either currently or in the past. Blanchard called this latter group autogynephilic -- "sexually aroused by the thought or image of oneself as a woman."

Two Types of Transsexuals

His studies revealed that gender dysphoric males who were primarily attracted to men -- those who were androphilic, or homosexual relative to their biologic sex -- usually reported having been rather feminine as children (Blanchard, 1988). They presented clinically at a fairly young age, about 26 years on average (Blanchard et al, 1987). Only about 15% of them gave any history of sexual arousal with cross-dressing (Blanchard, 1985). And generally they did not tend to be sexually aroused by fantasies of simply being female (Blanchard, 1989b).

Blanchard's other category of gender dysphoric males included those attracted primarily to women (gynephilic, or heterosexual relative to their biologic sex); those attracted to both women and men (bisexual); and those with little attraction to other persons of either sex (anallophilic, "not attracted to other people"). Blanchard (1988) found that the males in this combined group reported less childhood femininity than those in the androphilic group; often they seemed to have been unremarkably masculine as children. They presented for initial evaluation at a somewhat older age, about 34 years on average (Blanchard et al, 1987). About 75% of them admitted to a history sexual arousal with crossdressing (Blanchard, 1985). And, most significantly for Blanchard's theory, they were far more likely than those in the androphilic group to be sexually aroused by autogynephilic fantasies, that is, simply by the fantasy of being women (Blanchard, 1989b).

There is good reason to believe that among the non-androphilic subjects in the combined group, the actual number with some history of sexual arousal to crossdressing was probably considerably higher than 75%. In a related study, Blanchard and colleagues (1985) demonstrated that among gender dysphoric males who were heterosexual relative to their biologic sex, the tendency to deny sexual arousal to crossdressing was highly correlated with the tendency to otherwise describe oneself in a socially approved way, as measured by the Crowne-Marlowe Social Desirability Scale. Gender dysphoric males who were homosexual relative to their biologic sex did not show such a tendency. This provided a basis for believing that some of the combined group subjects might not be reporting their history accurately. Furthermore, Blanchard and colleagues (1986) demonstrated using penile plethysmography that many gynephilic transsexuals who denied sexual arousal to crossdressing actually did become aroused while listening to spoken descriptions of crossdressing scenarios. It therefore seems plausible to assume that among the non- androphilic subjects, a history of sexual arousal with crossdressing or cross-gender fantasy was probably nearly universal.

Transsexuality as a Paraphilia

Blanchard considered autogynephilia to be a type of paraphilia, or unusual sexual arousal pattern. Paraphilias are defined in DSM-IV as "recurrent, intense sexual urges, fantasies, or behaviors that involve unusual objects, activities, or situations and cause clinical distress or impairment in functioning" (American Psychiatric Association, 1994; p. 493). Paraphilias are often socially stigmatized, despite that fact that many are entirely harmless. In one of his later papers, Blanchard (1993b) amended his formulation somewhat, and suggested that autogynephilia could also be considered a sexual orientation:

autogynephilia might be better characterized as an orientation than as a paraphilia. The term orientation encompasses behavior, correlated with sexual behavior but distinct from it, that may ultimately have a greater impact on the life of the individual. For homosexual and heterosexual men, such correlated behavior includes courtship, love, and cohabitation with a partner of the preferred sex; for autogynephilic men, it includes the desire to achieve, with clothing, hormones, or surgery, an appearance like the preferred self-image of their erotic fantasies. (p. 306)

Blanchard (1991) formally distinguished four different types of autogynephilia in gender dysphoric males, although most of his subjects demonstrated more than one type. The first type was transvestic autogynephilia, in which the arousal was to the act or fantasy of wearing women's clothing. The second was behavioral autogynephilia, in which the arousal was to the act or fantasy of engaging in some behavior regarded as feminine; this behavior could range from knitting with other women to having sexual intercourse with a male. (The later case, according to Blanchard's formulation, was different from genuine androphilia, because the arousal was not to the male partner per se, but to engaging in a behavior regarded as typically female.) The third type was physiologic autogynephilia, in which the arousal was to fantasies such as being pregnant, menstruating, or breast-feeding. The fourth type was anatomic autogynephilia, in which the arousal was to the fantasy of having a woman's body, or aspects of one such as breasts or a vulva.

Autogynephilia and SRS

It was entirely predictable, Blanchard felt, that biologic males who experienced sexual arousal from the idea of having a woman's body would in fact seek to acquire or inhabit such a body. And his research subsequently confirmed that his subjects with the anatomic type of autogynephilia were the ones most interested in physical transformation, that is, in sex reassignment surgery. He summarized his theory this way (Blanchard, 1991):

Autogynephilia takes a variety of forms. Some men are most aroused sexually by the idea of wearing women's clothes, and they are primarily interested in wearing women's clothes. Some men are most aroused sexually by the idea of having a woman's body, and they are most interested in acquiring a woman's body. Viewed in this light, the desire for sex reassignment surgery of the latter group appears as logical as the desire of heterosexual men to marry wives, the desire of homosexual men to establish permanent relationships with male partners, and perhaps the desire of other paraphilic men to bond with their paraphilic objects in ways no one has thought to observe." (pp. 245-246)

I continue to regard this as one of the most brilliant and insightful analyses in the entire clinical literature devoted to transsexuality.

When Blanchard theorized that autogynephilic sexual desire can sometimes lead to a person to seek sex reassignment, he was referring to sexual desire in a broad sense. Sexual desire can, for example, be experienced even when there is no overt genital arousal. Blanchard was quite aware that his theory of autogynephilic transsexuality as an outgrowth of sexual desire would have to explain why the transsexual impulse persists even when genital arousal is reduced or absent. Many of us with a history of sexual arousal to crossdressing or autogynephilic fantasy report that while our overt sexual excitement diminishes over time, our desire for sex reassignment surgery usually continues and even intensifies. And when we autogynephilic transsexuals take estrogen, our interest in genital sexuality is often diminished, but our desire for sex reassignment usually is not. Such observations seemed to pose a problem for Blanchard's theory. He rationalized this apparent contradiction by suggesting that after a period of time, stimuli which have been experienced as sexually exciting come to be regarded as rewarding and desirable in their own right, even when they no longer evoke intense genital arousal (Blanchard, 1991). Again using the analogy of heterosexual marriage, he pointed out that husbands often continue to experience intense emotional connection to their wives, even after their initial intense sexual attraction has diminished or completely disappeared. Although this explanation is plausible, we shall see that there are other explanations as well.

Blanchard's theory does not contradict the statements of many transsexuals, including autogynephilic ones, that they find sex reassignment gratifying in many important non-sexual ways. We can accept these reports as accurate, and still simultaneously accept the idea that, for some of us, sexual desire was the origin of our transsexual impulse. Many of us find that learning to embody such feminine traits as gentleness, empathy, nurturance, and grace improves the quality of our lives, and simply makes us better human beings. Even those of us who acknowledge autogynephilic feelings often discover a number of genuine non-sexual reasons for wanting to undergo sex reassignment. Therefore it becomes easy, and not necessarily inaccurate, to tell ourselves and everyone else that we genuinely did want to transition for reasons that had nothing to do with sexual desire. Yet, many of us would probably have to admit that sexual desire was at least originally at the heart of our wish to transition.

Critiques of Blanchard's Theory

Not surprisingly, Blanchard's autogynephilia theory has met with some criticisms. I will consider four of these: First, while a history of autogynephilic eroticism and the desire for sex reassignment may sometimes be correlated, it is difficult if not impossible to demonstrate that the former invariably motivates or "drives" the latter. Second, a history of autogynephilic eroticism keeps cropping up in some androphilic transsexuals as well as in many gynephilic ones; this suggests that autogynephilia may sometimes be an effect rather than a cause of gender dysphoria, or an effect of some related phenomenon, such as childhood gender non-conformity that is experienced as ego-dystonic. Third, while transsexuals with a history of autogynephilic eroticism usually report less childhood femininity than do androphilic transsexuals, they often report more childhood femininity or gender non-conformity than do other boys. This suggests that long- standing discomfort with the male gender role may provide part of the impetus to seeking sex reassignment in some males with a history of autogynephilia, just as it presumably does in many androphilic transsexuals. Finally, some transsexuals who freely admit to autogynephilic eroticism report experiencing gender dysphoria long before they experienced autogynephilia. This suggests that perhaps gender dysphoria may predispose to the development of autogynephilia, rather than the reverse, and that some other factor -- possibly whether gender dysphoria is experienced as ego-syntonic or ego-dystonic -- may determine whether autogynephilic eroticism develops.

Critique: Autogynephilia as "Motive"

First, concerning motivation: Blanchard's research leaves little doubt that in some males, a history of autogynephilic arousal, and particularly anatomic autogynephilia, is correlated with the desire for sex reassignment. But to go from correlation to attribution of motivation is more problematic. Blanchard's measures of sexual arousal to crossdressing (Blanchard, 1985) and of autogynephilia (Blanchard, 1989b) did not distinguish between past and current sexual feelings. In virtually every case, his questionnaire items concerning these topics were of the form, "Have you ever experienced..." (emphasis added). This makes it likely that some of his subjects with a history of autogynephilic arousal might no longer have been experiencing autogynephilia at the time they applied for sex reassignment, and perhaps might not have experienced autogynephilic sexual desires for many years. It is difficult to plausibly attribute motive to a desire that is no longer being subjectively experienced. Blanchard (1991) acknowledged as much when he wrote:

in later years, however, autogynephilic sexual arousal may diminish or disappear, while the transsexual wish remains or grows even stronger. Such histories are often produced by gender dysphoric patients, but one does not have to rely on self-report to accept that the transsexual motive may attain, or inherently possess, some independence from autogynephilia. (p. 248; emphasis added)
This viewpoint seems to have been largely ignored in Blanchard's subsequent papers, however.

It is especially problematic to suggest, as I have done in past writings, that autogynephilic eroticism constitutes a motive for seeking sex reassignment -- except, of course, in the case of the many transsexuals who freely admit to this themselves. As sociologist C. Wright Mills (1940) observed, motives are nothing more or less than social communications. It is speculative, and ultimately unproductive, to theorize that the verbal explanations we call motives in some way represent, or at times misrepresent, objective internal psychological states or "drives." Such ideas cannot be empirically tested. It is not possible to objectively determine what a person's "real" motive might be, or whether a person's stated motive is genuine. As Mills wrote, "There is no way to plumb behind verbalization into an individual and directly check our motive- mongering..." (p. 910). He further observed that when we doubt or challenge a person's stated motive, all we are really saying is that their explanation does not fall within the vocabulary of motives that we regard as usual or plausible in such situations. (My thanks to Dr. Richard Ekins for recommending Mills' seminal paper.)

With the exception of the passage quoted above, Blanchard does not himself use the term "motive" in describing the relationship between autogynephilia and the pursuit of sex reassignment. However, his discussions of the issue, and especially his statement that non- androphilic males who seek physical feminization are "driven to achieve that appearance" by autogynephilia (Blanchard, 1993a; p. 74), leave little doubt that he sees autogynephilic eroticism as a genuine motivating factor, and not simply as a correlated feature. While this is consistent with the self-reports of some transsexuals, autogynephilia is not the only explanation for gender transition in persons with a history of autogynephilic arousal, as we shall see.

Critique: Autogynephilia in Androphiles

A second objection to Blanchard's theory is that a history of autogynephilic eroticism is consistently obtained from some apparently androphilic transsexuals, as well as from a great many gynephilic ones. This suggests that autogynephilia is not exclusively a misdirected form of heterosexuality, although plausibly this might be true some or even most of the time. The existence of autogynephilic eroticism in androphilic gender dysphoric males suggests that autogynephilia is only a symptom, and need not inflexibly define a whole category of persons. In particular, it suggests that putative dichotomies such as "younger/feminine/androphilic" versus "older/less feminine/autogynephilic," while possibly useful as generalizations, are over-simplifications of a more complicated reality. Does autogynephilia typically arise by a different mechanism in androphilic transsexuals than in most gynephilic transsexuals? If so, then perhaps autogynephilia is sometimes merely an epiphenomenon, more a symptom than a signifier. The more interestingly possibility is that autogynephilia might arise by the same mechanism in androphilic and gynephilic transsexuals. At present we can only conjecture about what such a common mechanism might be, but one obvious candidate suggests itself: childhood gender dysphoria that is experienced as ego-dystonic and that partially interferes with the development of normal erotic interests in other persons. I will return to this topic later.

Are some genuinely androphilic transsexuals also genuinely autogynephilic? It is difficult to avoid the conclusion that some are. In his earliest paper concerning these issues, Blanchard (1985) found that 15% of his androphilic subjects reported a history of sexual arousal to crossdressing. Bentler (1976) likewise observed that 23% of his homosexual (androphilic) transsexual subjects admitted to sexual arousal with crossdressing. Freund and colleagues (1982) found an even higher figure: 31% of their androphilic subjects gave such a history. In a later paper by Blanchard and colleagues (1987), 10% of the androphilic subjects also reported a history of sexual arousal to crossdressing. Blanchard (1989b) subsequently described the responses of androphilic and non- androphilic subjects on the Core Autogynephilia Scale, which measures sexual arousal to the thought or image of having a female body. The non- androphilic subjects endorsed more items, an average of 5.4 items out of a possible 8; but the androphilic subjects endorsed an average of 2.2 items, hardly a null response. Standard deviations averaged around 3.0 items, indicating substantial overlap between groups, as well as considerable anatomic autogynephilia among some of the 117 androphilic subjects. Leavitt and Berger (1990) studied 81 male-to-female transsexuals classified as androphilic using Blanchard's own Androphilia Scale; 36% of these admitted to a history of sexual arousal to crossdressing. Even considering only those subjects who were actively androphilic (thus eliminating those who might be "asexual" or anallophilic), 24% of this group also reported a history of sexual arousal to crossdressing.

Blanchard rationalized the reports of sexual arousal to crossdressing in androphilic subjects by proposing that some of those who admitted to autogynephilia were probably not genuinely androphilic, but were really gynephilic persons who had lied about their sexual orientation in order to appear more "classically" transsexual (Blanchard, 1985). But if this was so, why did they not also lie about their sexual arousal to crossdressing? Blanchard and colleagues (1985) demonstrated that non-androphilic gender dysphoric males who tended to describe themselves in socially approved ways were even more likely to deny a history of sexual arousal to crossdressing than they were to deny current sexual interest in women. And what about the possibility that some of the androphilic transsexuals who denied sexual arousal to crossdressing might have in fact demonstrated such arousal if tested with penile plethysmography -- just as some of their gynephilic counterparts had done? (Blanchard et al, 1986). Blanchard and colleagues seem not to have considered this possibility.

The existence of genuinely androphilic transsexuals who are also genuinely autogynephilic does not, of course, contradict Blanchard's theory concerning the importance of autogynephilia in the life paths of many gynephilic transsexuals. But it does call into question the meaning of autogynephilic eroticism, and it suggests that autogynephilia is only a symptom, the significance of which may vary from person to person.

Critique: Gender Non-Conformity in Autogynephiles

A third criticism of Blanchard's theory is that it fails to take into account another explanation for seeking sex reassignment, one that is frequently given by some transsexuals with a history of autogynephilic eroticism. This is their feeling of long-standing discomfort with the male gender role. Typically these transsexuals report a history of childhood gender non-conformity, which was usually less dramatic than that reported by androphilic transsexuals, but which was present nonetheless. However, unlike their androphilic counterparts, these individuals usually experienced their childhood gender non-conformity as ego-dystonic, and attempted to repress it. What are we to make of such self-reports?

For his part, Blanchard does not seem to believe that gynephilic transsexuals experience or display much femininity or gender non- conformity early in life. He described such persons as being "without outward signs of effeminacy" as children (Blanchard, 1990; p. 57), and he approvingly agreed with Person and Ovesey's contention (1974) that heterosexual transsexuals "are not effeminate in childhood" (Blanchard, 1988; p. 192).

But other experienced clinicians see autogynephilic persons, transsexual or otherwise, as experiencing a kind of genuine femininity, feminine self-image, or discomfort with the male gender role, which they seek to express for non-sexual reasons as well as for sexual ones. Langevin (1985), another clinician from the Clarke Institute, expressed the view that in some males with a history of sexual arousal to crossdressing, the crossdressing "is over-determined in that it serves orgasmic and gender needs. As drive level diminishes with age and sexual curiosity decreases, the gender need may remain strong and stable so that they now apply for surgery" (p. 214). Johnson and Hunt (1990) suggested that autogynephilic transsexuals experience real gender conflicts, just as androphilic transsexuals do; the former simply resolve these later in life. Levine (1993) thought that "crossdressing and ... autogynephilic fantasy are the external and internal manifestations of the same phenomenon -- the conscious experience of the self as at least partially female" (p. 135).

Blanchard's only paper that looked at childhood femininity in males seeking sex reassignment (Blanchard, 1988) considered retrospective self-reports by small numbers of androphilic, gynephilic, bisexual and asexual transsexuals. Because his aim was to explore similarities between the last three groups, Blanchard looked at only 64 of his pool of 256 subjects, selecting the 16 most prototypical examples within each of the four groups. Among these 64, the 16 exclusively androphilic subjects reported significantly more childhood femininity than those in the other three groups, who were not significantly different from each other. The limitations of this study should be obvious. Would the results have been the same if all the subjects had been included? How did the members of the three non-androphilic groups compare to male controls? Even if they reported less childhood femininity than the androphilic males, the non- androphilic males might still have reported substantially more childhood femininity than non-transsexual males. How much variability in childhood femininity existed within each group? Perhaps some of the gynephilic transsexuals were very feminine individually, even if the group as a whole was not.

Evidence from other sources is inconsistent concerning the extent of childhood femininity in non-androphilic transsexuals. Johnson and Hunt (1990), in a study of 25 male-to-female transsexuals, found a significant negative relationship between gynephilia and self-reported childhood femininity. But other researchers have found that some gynephilic transsexuals report substantial childhood femininity. Buhrich and McConaghy (1977) explored this topic in 12 late-onset transsexuals with a clear history of fetishistic arousal. Of these,

11 of 12 "always wanted to be a girl;"
10 of 12 cross-dressed prior to puberty;
8 of 12 avoided rough and tumble sports;
10 of 12 preferred girls' company and games;
7 of 12 were called "sissy" at school.
Similarly, Doorn et al (1994) studied childhood gendered behaviors in early-onset and late-onset transsexuals; the former were younger, and more likely to be androphilic. The early-onset transsexuals reported more frequent feminine play preferences as children, compared to the late-onset transsexuals; but the late-onset transsexuals had more frequent feminine play preferences than did a comparison of transvestites, or a group of normal male controls. There was also substantial variability in play preferences within all groups.

All these studies of childhood femininity in transsexuals relied on self-reports, and were thus subject to potential distortion, deliberate or otherwise. And there is reason to believe that gynephilic transsexuals may be more prone to such distortion than androphilic transsexuals. Blanchard and colleagues (1985) found that gynephilic gender dysphoric males who tended to describe themselves in socially approved ways also tended to report significantly more childhood femininity; androphilic males showed a much smaller and non-significant tendency to do this. Still, the hypothesis that at least some gynephilic transsexuals may experience significant childhood femininity or gender non-conformity, and that this may provide some impetus to gender transition, is virtually impossible to refute.

Critique: Gender Dysphoria Preceding Autogynephilia

A final objection to Blanchard's theory is that some transsexuals who freely admit to autogynephilic eroticism also consistently report that they experienced gender dysphoria long before they experienced autogynephilic arousal. This suggests that perhaps sometimes gender dysphoria may predispose to the development of autogynephilia, rather than the reverse. The term gender dysphoria denotes dissatisfaction with the sexed body and one's assigned sex (Laub and Fisk, 1974), with or without dissatisfaction with the gender role of that sex. Blanchard and colleagues (1987) suggested that in non-androphilic transsexuals, autogynephilic eroticism was primary, and was a "predisposing condition" (p. 149) for the later development of gender dysphoria . But the reverse could as easily be true, with gender dysphoria predisposing certain males -- mostly gynephilic, but perhaps some androphilic -- to the development of autogynephilia. Since we do not really understand how any erotic preference develops, we can only conjecture about how gender dysphoria might lead to autogynephilia. But one possibility is that cross-gender wishes may become eroticized when they are experienced as ego-dystonic and consequently partially interfere with the development of normal erotic interests in other persons.

It is true that, on average, gynephilic transsexuals report crossdressing for the first time -- often with sexual arousal -- almost simultaneously with experiencing their first cross-gender wish (Blanchard et al, 1987). This suggests a close link between autogynephilia and gender dysphoria. But this observation is not inconsistent with reports by some non-androphilic transsexuals that their feelings of gender dysphoria preceded their feelings of autogynephilic eroticism by several years. Seil (1996) has suggested that the principal distinction between the mostly-androphilic, mostly-early-transitioning group of transsexuals, and the mostly-gynephilic, mostly-late-transitioning group, is that former experience their gender dysphoria as ego-syntonic, while for the latter it is ego-dystonic. It is temping to hypothesize that autogynephilia may develop when ego-dystonic cross-gender feelings partially interfere with the development of normal erotic interests in other persons. This is consistent with the long-standing observation that autogynephilia partially competes with sexual interests in other persons (Blanchard, 1992). It would also explain the consistent finding of autogynephilic eroticism in some androphilic transsexuals, as previously noted.

Blanchard's Theory in Perspective

Although Blanchard's theory of autogynephilia has a few weaknesses and limitations, as we have seen, I find little reason to doubt the theory's fundamental soundness. In my opinion, it remains an impressive formulation, of great descriptive and predictive value. If nothing else, the way it has been embraced by hundreds of transsexual women, most of whom never before felt that their experience had been adequately described, is testimony to its value. But it is important to remember that some aspects of Blanchard's theory do allow for alternative explanations. And there are also a few well-documented observations that his theory cannot adequately explain, except by the unsatisfying explanation that persons who do not conform to the theory's predictions must be reporting inaccurately, or deliberately lying.

It is also important not to over-generalize from Blanchard's theory. He indisputably found statistical correlations, and highly significant ones, between several of the variables he examined. But this does not imply that the patterns he discovered will necessarily hold true in any particular individual case. It may be true in general that transsexuals who are primarily androphilic tend to transition earlier, to be more feminine as children, and to be less aroused by cross-gender fantasy that those who are primarily gynephilic. But I have no doubt that there are some gynephilic transsexuals who transition early, who are quite feminine as children, and who display little or no autogynephilic arousal. Likewise, I have no doubt that there are some androphilic transsexuals who transition late, who are not especially feminine as children, and who display substantial autogynephilic arousal.

We should also remember that theories like Blanchard's are simply scientific models for describing and predicting human behavior. Such theories say, in effect, that humans generally behave as if this or that were true. Blanchard's theory was designed to describe and predict individual behaviors; it was not designed to, and it cannot be expected to, describe or predict all the possible subjective experiences, interpretations, or meanings of those behaviors for the transsexual persons who engage in them. These highly personal, intrapsychic aspects of the transsexual journey are really beyond the scope of behavioral science, at least in its current form, and are perhaps better addressed in philosophical or spiritual terms. What is remarkable about Blanchard's autogynephila theory is that for some of us -- though certainly not for all -- it really does seem to "get inside our heads," and to provide an amazingly accurate description of our subjective experiences -- a mirror of what gender dysphoria and the process of seeking sex reassignment actually feels like to us.

More Support for the Theory

Despite nearly a decade of work, Blanchard never tested his ideas about autogynephilia in a group of postoperative male-to-female transsexuals. I explored the topic among a group of postoperative transsexual women at the 1996 and 1998 New Woman's Conferences, using an anonymous survey method. In 1996, ten of thirteen post-op women agreed that "self- feminization was erotic" for them (Lawrence, 1997). And seven of thirteen agreed that "self-feminization had been their primary erotic fantasy prior to transition." In 1998, five of eleven women agreed that before surgery, their "favorite erotic fantasy was that they had, or were acquiring, some features of a woman's body" (Lawrence, 1998). Additional evidence for the importance of autogynephilic fantasy in transsexuals who have undergone surgery comes from Maryann Schroder's unpublished Ph.D. thesis, "New Women" (Schroder, 1995). Five of her seventeen postoperative subjects described having been aroused by autogynephilic sexual fantasies prior to surgery. I have subsequently collected a great many individual narratives by transsexual women, attesting to the importance of autogynephilic eroticism in their life paths.

Why Has Autogynephilia Been Ignored?

Why is it that autogynephilia has received so little attention, even though it is readily reported by some transsexual women? I think there are several reasons. Among many transsexuals, autogynephilia is often considered not to be a respectable topic for discussion. Some transsexuals have a passionate dislike for the policies of the Clarke Institute, and tend to dismiss almost reflexively any research performed by its clinicians. Therefore, Blanchard's ideas are not often talked about; and when they are raised, they tend to get shouted down. Shame is undoubtedly another deterrent. It is probably just too threatening for many transsexuals to admit that they have had autogynephilic fantasies, and especially to admit that autogynephilic sexual desire may have been one of their motives for seeking sex reassignment. Most of us transsexual women want to be seen as "real" women, and this usually requires explaining our desire to transition as an outgrowth of our intrinsic internal femininity. Transsexuals who admit that their transitions were motivated by autogynephilic sexual desire may not be seen as real women, and may not even be seen as real transsexuals.

Therapists and surgeons probably have their own reasons for ignoring autogynephilia. Most therapists are accustomed to thinking about transsexuality using more traditional, gender-based formulations. The idea that there can be sexual motivations for transition may seem inconsistent with their paradigm; accordingly, they may think that autogynephilic sexual desire in transsexuals is rare and aberrant. And since their clients are often unwilling to talk about the subject, who can blame the therapists for thinking so? Moreover, most therapists and surgeons would probably find it difficult to acknowledge that when they give approval for sex reassignment surgery, or perform it, they are sometimes simply helping a transsexual woman act out her own paraphilic sexual script.

Is Autogynephilia an "Acceptable" Motive?

Personally, however, I do not find the idea that transsexual women sometimes seek reassignment surgery for sexual reasons to be especially problematic, even when their sexuality is essentially paraphilic. The real question is not what one's motivation is, but whether sex reassignment surgery improves the quality of one's life. The overall level of satisfaction following sex reassignment surgery is extremely high. And the evidence seems to demonstrate that those transsexual women whom one would expect to be autogynephilic -- late onset and sexually attracted to women -- tend to do about as well after surgery as those who present younger, and who are sexually attracted to men.

No one should be shocked to learn that there are people who want to modify their bodies, surgically and hormonally, primarily to enhance their sexual attractiveness to themselves or to others. This is a multi-billion dollar industry in the United States alone, and most of it does not involve transsexuals. We routinely cut, color, tattoo, pierce, augment, reduce, lift, tuck, rearrange and contour parts of our bodies to express our sexuality and to enhance our sexual satisfaction. Nor is this unique to modern Western culture, as any anthropologist can tell you. I am not suggesting that we should be complacent about SRS, or regard it as a casual undertaking. I am suggesting that, to my mind, having sexual motivations for seeking sex reassignment surgery does not pose any kind of unique ethical problem. Sexual motivations underlie a vast range of medical and surgical procedures which are routinely performed on non-transsexuals, and which we more or less take for granted.

Spiritual Implications of Blanchard's Theory

Let me close with a few brief comments on the spiritual implications of Blanchard's autogynephilia theory. The philosopher Friedrich Nietzsche (1886/1966; p. 86) wrote:

The great epochs of our life come when we gain the courage to rechristen our evil as what is best in us.
Nietzsche's observation is directly relevant to our individual responses to the autogynephilic eroticism that, for many of us, lies at the core of our transsexual desire. One of the most negative consequences of our Judeo- Christian heritage is our tendency to see anything sexual as evil, degraded, and unworthy. Other high cultures have taken a more balanced view of life. The Greeks, whom Nietzsche revered, understood the importance of expressing and integrating both the Apollonian and the Dionysian aspects of life. Likewise, many non-Western cultures have understood that sexual desire is the wellspring of our creativity, and of our emotional and physical vitality. They have understood that sexuality can be our most direct pathway to transcendence, and to the spiritual dimensions of life.

I think there has been an unfortunate tendency to see Blanchard's autogynephilia theory as pathologizing or degrading the transsexual experience. I don't see it that way at all. We have grown up hearing that our sexual desires are evil and unworthy, and many of us have come to believe it. But I believe, with Nietzsche, that to live fulfilling and authentic lives as transsexual persons, we must be willing to rechristen our sexual desire as that which is best and most profoundly human within us. We must honor our sexual desire as that which moves us most, as that which makes us feel most truly alive.

We need make no apology for deciding to rebuild our lives around the most powerful feelings we know. I think it is an act of existential courage to honor our deepest feelings by giving them a central place in our lives. By transitioning to live as women, we can give tangible expression to our sexual feelings -- feelings that many of us have tried too long to suppress and deny. And although our paths can be challenging, the benefits in terms of vitality and inner peace can be profound.

Although I usually write about transsexuality as a physician and a scientist, I can also appreciate and embrace the views of authors like Susan Stryker (1994) and Rachel Pollack (1992), who write about the transsexual journey from a more spiritual perspective. To me, the erotic desire at the core of autogynephilic transsexuality seems like just another aspect of what Rachel calls the "divine force" that leads us to abandon ourselves to our bodies' desire, and what Susan refers to as the "enlivening power" of our identification with Nature's dark, creative chaos.

When we recognize and honor the autogynephilic feelings within ourselves, we do not declare ourselves sick or debased. Rather, we affirm that a life built on passion and authenticity is truly a life worth living.

_________________________________

REFERENCES:

American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (4th edition). Washington, DC: Author; 522-523.

Blanchard, R. (1985). Typology of male-to-female transsexualism. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 14, 247-261.

Blanchard, R. (1988). Nonhomosexual gender dysphoria. Journal of Sex Research, 24, 188-193.

Blanchard, R. (1989a). The classification and labeling of nonhomosexual gender dysphorias. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 18, 315-334.

Blanchard, R. (1989b). The concept of autogynephilia and the typology of male gender dysphoria. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 177, 616- 623.

Blanchard, R. (1990). Gender identity disorders in adult men. In R. Blanchard & B. Steiner (Eds.), Clinical Management of Gender Identity Disorders in Children and Adults. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 49-76.

Blanchard, R. (1991). Clinical observations and systematic studies of autogynephilia. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 17, 235-251.

Blanchard, R. (1992). Nonmonotonic relation of autogynephilia and heterosexual attraction. Journal of Abnonmal Psychology, 101, 271-276.

Blanchard, R. (1993a). The she-male phenomenon and the concept of partial autogynephilia. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 19, 69-76.

Blanchard, R. (1993b). Partial versus complete autogynephilia and gender dysphoria. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 19, 301-307.

Blanchard, R. (1993c). Varieties of autogynephilia and their relationship to gender dysphoria. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 22, 241-251.

Blanchard, R., Clemmensen, L., & Steiner, B. (1985). Social desirability response set and systematic distortion in the self-report of adult male gender patients. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 14, 505-516.

Blanchard, R., Clemmensen, L., & Steiner, B. (1987). Heterosexual and homosexual gender dysphoria. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 16, 139-152.

Blanchard, R., Rachansky, I., & Steiner, B. (1986). Phallometric detection of fetishistic arousal in heterosexual male cross-dressers. Journal of Sex Research, 22, 452-462.

Doorn, C., Poortinga, J., & Verschoor, A. (1994). Cross-gender identity in transvestites and male transsexuals. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 23, 185-201.

Freund, K., Steiner, B., & Chan, S. (1982). Two types of cross-gender identity. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 11, 49-63.

Johnson, S., & Hunt, D. (1990). The relationship of male transsexual typology to psychosocial adjustment. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19, 349-360.

Laub, D., & Fisk, N. (1974). A rehabilitation program for gender dysphoria syndrome by surgical sex change. Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, 53, 388-403.

Lawrence, A. A. (1997, June). Life after surgery: Questions and answers from the 1996 New Woman's Conference. Paper presented at the Second International Congress on Sex and Gender Issues, Philadelphia, PA., June 20.

Lawrence, A. (1998). Unpublished data.

Leavitt, F., & Berger, J. (1990). Clinical patterns among male transsexual candidates with an erotic interest in males. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 19, 491-505.

Levine, S. (1993). Gender-disturbed males. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 19, 131-141.

Mills, C. (1940). Situated actions and vocabularies of motive. American Sociological Review, 5, 904-913.

Nietzsche, F. (1886/1966). Beyond Good and Evil. New York: Vintage.

Person, E., & Ovesey, L. (1974). The transsexual syndrome in males: secondary transsexualism. American Journal of Psychotherapy, 28, 174-193.

Pollack, R. (1992, Fall). Abandonment to the body's desire. Rites of Passage, 19.

Schroder, M. (1995). New women: Sexological outcomes of gender reassignment surgery. Unpublished Ph.D. thesis, Institute for Advanced Study of Human Sexuality, San Francisco, CA.

Seil, D. (1996). Transsexuals: The boundary of sexual identity and gender. In R. Cabaj & T. Stein (Eds.), Textbook of Homosexuality and Mental Health. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 743-762.

Stryker, S. (1994). My words to Victor Frankenstein above the village of Chamounix: Performing transgender rage. GLQ, 1, 227-254.


Return To Sexuality Index Page

Return To TWR Main Page

© 2000 by Anne A. Lawrence, M.D., Ph.D. All rights reserved.